I’ve just read
Thomas Leitch’s very generous review of Patrick Cattrysse’s DESCRIPTIVE
ADAPTATION STUDIES published in the latest issue of ADAPTATION, where he
accuses the author of being prescriptive, as well as being in favor of a
“science-based discipline,” suggesting, perhaps, that there might be some
conclusions to which everyone, regardless of context and culture, could
subscribe. Leitch himself prefers to
describe adaptation as “endlessly debatable, revisitable, [full
of] adaptable questions, insights, and leaps of faith,” even though he
describes Cattrysse’s work as indispensable as well as infuriating. The review can be accessed at http://adaptation.oxfordjournals.org/content/current.
I don’t want to
comment on Cattrysse’s work anymore (I reviewed it for Literature/Film Quarterly and referred to it frequently in
subsequent blog-posts and essays; but what does warrant further comment is his
assertion that any discipline can be “science-based.” I wonder what that term actively signifies:
does it mean that it consists of a series of indisputable precepts beyond
negotiation? Or can adaptation studies
be reconfigured as a series of experiments designed to prove a particular
theorem? Will we be able to divide
further essays for Adaptation or Literature/Film Quarterly into sections
in a fashion similar to those used in pedagogical studies, with particular
sections devoted to “problem,” “literature review,” “application,” or
conclusion”?
Or is the entire
notion of “science-based” disciplines the invention of critics desirous to
prove that what they are doing represents an important contribution to their
specific discipline? I grew up with the
work of F. R. Leavis, a controversial figure of mid-twentieth century British
literary criticism, who insisted on critical objectivity; any poem, or other
text, could be analyzed with scientific precision, producing a series of
indisputable conclusions on content and form.
This is how I learned to “do” practical criticism; by dividing my
textual analyses into content and form, I could understand in minute detail
precisely what the writer was trying to communicate, with a depth of knowledge
denied to ordinary readers.
I’ve just finished
reading Ahmet Hamdi Tanpınar’s satirical novel The Time Regulation Institute.
Originally published in 1961 under the Turkish title Saatleri Ayarlama Enstitüsü,
it is a wildly funny satire of bureaucracies, where legions of people spend
most of their time doing nothing. One of
Tanpınar’s major concerns lies in emphasizing the difficulties of separating
truth from fiction: people believe what they want to with little concern for
veracity. The hero of the novel is an
illiterate with little or no self-reliance; catapulted into prominence in the
Time Regulation Institute, he has a whole past invented on his behalf that
transforms him into an ideal husband, Stakhanovite worker and intellectual
visionary. No one bothers to question
him, even when the Institute collapses.
Tanpınar’s satire encourages us to consider precisely
how and why certain discourses are perceived as authoritative in preference to
others. In the case of the novel’s hero,
it is his status at the centre of the company that renders him an authoritative
figure. The fact he is manifestly
unqualified for the task doesn’t really matter; in fact, it proves a positive
advantage based on the principle that ignorance is bliss. I don’t want to press the analogy too far,
but it seems to me that the reason why Leavis’s (or Cattrysse’s) assertions are
given credence is for a similar reason; it’s not what they are saying that’s
important, but the status of the
speakers themselves. If we accept what
they are saying, then perhaps we might share their status in the future.
What has this discussion got to do with adaptation
studies? Theoretically speaking, not
much. But what Cattrysse’s comment does
reveal is the presence of a battleground, where critics and theorists of
different disciplines are competing for recognition of the kind Leavis enjoyed
half a century ago. The stereotypical
image of scholars beavering away in their ivory towers developing ideas is
nothing but a myth; adaptation studies specialists – myself included – are as
publicity-conscious as anyone writing in the public sphere.
But perhaps this is no bad thing. The more scholars contribute interventions,
the more possibilities emerge for theoretically informed debate – not closed
debate over “scientific” principles, as Cattrysse might assume, but debate
between colleagues from various disciplinary origins as to how the discipline
might advance as a self-contained unit, or how adaptation studies might be used
to advance other disciplines. We need
authors like Tanpınar to skewer our pretensions, and thereby understand how scholarly
positions are constructed and reconstructed across cultures. I value Cattrysse’s comment, if only to show
how the critic-as-authoritative-figure is an ideological construct that needs
to be debunked.
Great stuff, Laurence--interesting and thought provoking. As always. We might see Cattrysse's assertion of adaptation studies as a science-based discipline as a counter-balance reaction to the field's 20th-century history of neglecting to try to establish what Ray calls "a presiding poetics." And we should remember that science is, at its core, also "endlessly debatable, revisitable, [full of] adaptable questions, insights, and leaps of faith." Perhaps, in its most productive light, "science-based" is a buzz phrase that accentuates the need for more systematic theoretical approaches to adaptation studies--a call to move, once and for all, away from rootless studies that use nothing more than the critic's evaluative judgments to beg the questions and prescribe the answers about what adaptation does.
ReplyDeleteAt the same time, as you point out, there is a considerably less productive light that might be shining on the term "science-based discipline." We definitely need to be on guard against these weird rearings of the New Critic's head in the field. It seems to me that some nooks and crannies in adaptation studies are serving as hidey holes for dormant New Critical urges. Such closed-system approaches definitely tend to shut down cross pollination and cause colleagues from other disciplines to, regrettably, keep their distance.